(C) PLOS One
This story was originally published by PLOS One and is unaltered.
. . . . . . . . . .



Towards a great ape dictionary: Inexperienced humans understand common nonhuman ape gestures [1]

['Kirsty E. Graham', 'Wild Minds Lab', 'School Of Psychology', 'Neuroscience', 'University Of St Andrews', 'St Andrews', 'Scotland', 'United Kingdom', 'Catherine Hobaiter']

Date: 2023-02

In the comparative study of human and nonhuman communication, ape gesturing provided the first demonstrations of flexible, intentional communication outside human language. Rich repertoires of these gestures have been described in all ape species, bar one: us. Given that the majority of great ape gestural signals are shared, and their form appears biologically inherited, this creates a conundrum: Where did the ape gestures go in human communication? Here, we test human recognition and understanding of 10 of the most frequently used ape gestures. We crowdsourced data from 5,656 participants through an online game, which required them to select the meaning of chimpanzee and bonobo gestures in 20 videos. We show that humans may retain an understanding of ape gestural communication (either directly inherited or part of more general cognition), across gesture types and gesture meanings, with information on communicative context providing only a marginal improvement in success. By assessing comprehension, rather than production, we accessed part of the great ape gestural repertoire for the first time in adult humans. Cognitive access to an ancestral system of gesture appears to have been retained after our divergence from other apes, drawing deep evolutionary continuity between their communication and our own.

Introduction

Regarded by philosophers and scientists alike as the cognitive capacity most critical to human uniqueness [1], the apparent discontinuity between human language and nonhuman communication has been argued to present an evolutionary puzzle. However, more and more research has started to unveil language’s deep phylogenetic roots: from the way other species combine signals to change the meaning (we use “meaning” in this article to refer to signal functions and Apparently Satisfactory Outcomes; [2,3]) of an utterance [4]; to their use of social inference in communication [5]; to how behavioural and social contexts seem to disambiguate signal meanings [6]. Nevertheless, many species’ communication is based on the exchange of specific, detailed information: Alarm calls, for example, can encode combinatory information on both the type and proximity of a predator [7,8]. While a rich source of information, these signals typically exist as a fixed response to stimuli, produced irrespective of a recipient’s attention or interest, or even whether a recipient is there [9]. Humans produce these types of signals too. Picking up a too-hot pan from the cooker, we might give an involuntary yelp, shake our hand, and/or make a facial grimace of pain. Any potential recipients around receive useful information from these signals: The pan is hot! But we did not yelp, shake, or grimace with the goal of communicating, we’d have done it whether someone was there or not. Language is different. We choose whether to tell someone who was out of the room to “watch out, the pan is hot.” We can use it in the absence of the stimuli that we were originally responding to. We would stop using it once our recipient indicated that they understood. We might even use it to talk to ourselves.

Fundamentally, with language, we do more than broadcast information; we intend to communicate a goal to a partner we recognise as having their own behaviour, goals, and knowledge. Human languages’ intentional nature takes it beyond sharing information: It communicates meaning [10–13]. This fundamental property is very rarely observed in other species [9,14], and when it is, it is typically restricted to one or two signals used in a highly specified way [15,16]. Nevertheless, the emergence of intentional communication through a single recent genetic leap in the human lineage remains implausible; instead, precursor abilities were likely present in the communication of our evolutionary ancestors and should be shared among modern ape species today [17].

Strikingly, great ape gestures are used in this language-like way: Rich systems of over 80 signals deployed communicate everyday goals (for example, [2,18–25]), and ape gesturing has been suggested to be an important scaffold in the evolution of human language [26,27]. Great ape repertoires show substantial overlap across species, including overlap among ape species more distantly related than chimpanzees, bonobos, and humans [28–32]. As a result, we would expect humans to retain the use of this system of ape gestural communication; but, to date, the use of naturalistic ape gestures appeared to be absent in human communication. Humans are highly gestural, deploying deictic, iconic, conventional, co-speech/co-sign, among other kinds of gestures. However, this itself is part of what makes studying gestural overlap between adult humans and other apes challenging. Gestures shared with other apes may be masked by the myriad ways that people signal with their hands and body. From pointing to pantomime, language-competent humans regularly employ gestures that accompany [33] and may even create [34] language; highly variable across cultures, they are rarely used to independently convey the core goal of the communication and do not map closely onto those employed by nonhuman apes. Unpicking gestures from the great ape repertoire in naturalistic adult human gesturing may not be impossible, but it will take a substantial collaborative effort to one day do so. In the meantime, there are other methods at our disposal. A recent study suggested that gestures from the “ape repertoire” may not be completely absent: Before language emerges, preverbal 1- to 2-year-old human infants were found to deploy over 50 gestures from the ape repertoire [35]. Given the available movements and body parts, there are well over 1,000 potential gesture forms that could be produced with the ape body, but apes only use approximately 12% of these [36]. Thus, any overlap between species is very unlikely to be trivial. Here, we provide the first test of the hypothesis that language-competent adult humans still share access to “family-typical” great ape gesture.

We employ a method regularly used in studies of nonhuman primate communication, a “play-back” experiment, in which recipient behaviour is analysed following exposure to a signal [37,38]. This type of comprehension study has historically been employed to test nonhuman species on comprehension of human language [39,40], but here we flip the paradigm to test humans on nonhuman communication. Of course, our experimental paradigm is more conventional in the human psychology literature and has the advantages that, with humans rather than nonhumans, we are able to conduct tests with untrained participants and to use text responses in this match-to-sample type paradigm. While language-competent humans seem to no longer typically produce gestures from the ape repertoire (or that these gestures may be masked by other common human-typical gesturing), the presence of a signal in an individual’s communicative repertoire can also be shown through their comprehension of it [30]. We conducted an online experiment to crowdsource whether adult human subjects understand the meaning of gestures produced by nonhuman apes. The experiment was presented in Gorilla.sc (www.gorilla.sc; [41]); a full preview and all importable sheets are available through Gorilla Open Materials (https://app.gorilla.sc/openmaterials/344409); and video data files are available at the Great Ape Dictionary on YouTube (https://tinyurl.com/greatapedictionary). Participants were randomly allocated to two conditions: those who viewed gesture videos only (Video only), and those who viewed gesture videos with a brief, one-line description of context (Context). Each video was accompanied by a simple illustration of the gesture to assist inexperienced viewers in identifying the gesture action (https://greatapedictionary.ac.uk/gesture-videos2/). From a set of 40 videos, each participant saw 20 videos with examples of ape gesture (10 chimpanzee, 10 bonobo). Videos were cut to show only the gesture, eliminating any behaviour before or after communication.

We selected the 10 most common gesture types for which we were previously able to confirm “meaning” in both chimpanzees and bonobos, determined by recipient responses that consistently satisfy the signaller [19]. Chimpanzees and bonobos are humans’ closest living relatives (we are also theirs, with the split from humans more recent than the last common ancestor shared between Pan and Gorilla; [42]). While in principle, given the overlap in gesture repertoires across all apes [28], we would predict that gorilla and orangutan gestures may also be salient to humans, the meanings for gestures in these ape species are not yet established.

Some gestures are used towards a single meaning (i.e., recipients consistently respond in the same way to that gesture), whereas others are used towards two or more meanings [2,19]. For example, the Big Loud Scratch is used to initiate grooming (meaning = “Groom me”), while Object Shake is used to initiate copulation (meaning = “Let’s have sex”), to initiate grooming (meaning = “Groom me”), and to increase distance between signaller and recipient (meaning = “Move away”). The correct meaning for a gesture video stimulus was assigned based on the specific meaning used for that instance of communication, rather than in general for that gesture type. Six of the chimpanzee and 7 of the bonobo gesture types had a single meaning, and 4 chimpanzee and 3 bonobo gestures types had multiple meanings. For these ambiguous gesture types, participants viewed one instance where the correct outcome was the primary meaning (the most common recipient response to that gesture type), and one instance where the correct outcome was the alternate meaning (the second most common recipient response), and in both cases were given the primary and alternate meanings as possible answers. Some of the gesture types, for example, Directed Push, have different primary and alternate meanings, for example, “Climb on my back” for bonobos and “Move to a new position” for chimpanzees. For these, as for ambiguous gestures, we expect participants to answer with the correct response for the specific video.

[END]
---
[1] Url: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/article?id=10.1371/journal.pbio.3001939

Published and (C) by PLOS One
Content appears here under this condition or license: Creative Commons - Attribution BY 4.0.

via Magical.Fish Gopher News Feeds:
gopher://magical.fish/1/feeds/news/plosone/