(C) PLOS One [1]. This unaltered content originally appeared in journals.plosone.org.
Licensed under Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license.
url:
https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/licenses-and-copyright
------------
Consolidation and maintenance of long-term memory involve dual functions of the developmental regulator Apterous in clock neurons and mushroom bodies in the Drosophila brain
['Show Inami', 'Department Of Biological Sciences', 'Tokyo Metropolitan University', 'Tokyo', 'Tomohito Sato', 'Yuto Kurata', 'Yuki Suzuki', 'Toshihiro Kitamoto', 'Department Of Anesthesia', 'Neuroscience']
Date: 2021-12
Memory is initially labile but can be consolidated into stable long-term memory (LTM) that is stored in the brain for extended periods. Despite recent progress, the molecular and cellular mechanisms underlying the intriguing neurobiological processes of LTM remain incompletely understood. Using the Drosophila courtship conditioning assay as a memory paradigm, here, we show that the LIM homeodomain (LIM-HD) transcription factor Apterous (Ap), which is known to regulate various developmental events, is required for both the consolidation and maintenance of LTM. Interestingly, Ap is involved in these 2 memory processes through distinct mechanisms in different neuronal subsets in the adult brain. Ap and its cofactor Chip (Chi) are indispensable for LTM maintenance in the Drosophila memory center, the mushroom bodies (MBs). On the other hand, Ap plays a crucial role in memory consolidation in a Chi-independent manner in pigment dispersing factor (Pdf)-containing large ventral–lateral clock neurons (l-LNvs) that modulate behavioral arousal and sleep. Since disrupted neurotransmission and electrical silencing in clock neurons impair memory consolidation, Ap is suggested to contribute to the stabilization of memory by ensuring the excitability of l-LNvs. Indeed, ex vivo imaging revealed that a reduced function of Ap, but not Chi, results in exaggerated Cl − responses to the inhibitory neurotransmitter gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) in l-LNvs, indicating that wild-type (WT) Ap maintains high l-LNv excitability by suppressing the GABA response. Consistently, enhancing the excitability of l-LNvs by knocking down GABA A receptors compensates for the impaired memory consolidation in ap null mutants. Overall, our results revealed unique dual functions of the developmental regulator Ap for LTM consolidation in clock neurons and LTM maintenance in MBs.
Funding: This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number 21H02528 to T.S. (
https://www.jsps.go.jp/ ), Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas (Singularity Biology;
http://singularity-bio.jp/ ) from the Ministry of Education, Science, Sports and Culture of Japan (21H00434 to T.S.), and SEI Group CSR Foundation to T.S. (
https://www.sei-group-csr.or.jp/ ). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Here, we provide evidence indicating that Ap and Chi in MBs are essential for maintaining courtship LTM, whereas Ap in Pdf neurons is necessary for memory consolidation through the regulation of the appropriate Cl − responses to gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) in a Chi-independent manner.
In this study, using a courtship conditioning assay, we examined if and how Ap in the adult brain is involved in Drosophila learning and memory. In the courtship conditioning assay, male flies are paired with unreceptive mated females, which give sufficient stresses (e.g., courtship-inhibiting cues and sexual rejection) to males to interfere with mating success (conditioning). After being conditioned, their memory is subsequently observed as the suppression of male courtship even toward virgin females. Previous studies have identified several genes involved in memory consolidation to establish courtship LTM. These include (1) Cyclic-AMP response element-binding protein B (CrebB) encoding a transcriptional factor [ 4 ]; (2) Notch encoding a transmembrane receptor [ 26 ]; (3) orb2 encoding an mRNA-binding protein [ 27 ]; and (4) Ecdysone Receptor encoding a nuclear hormone receptor [ 28 ]. Since these gene products regulate protein expression, it is most likely that de novo protein synthesis is essential for memory consolidation to establish courtship LTM. CrebB and Notch are also essential for Drosophila LTM induced by olfactory associative learning [ 4 – 6 , 29 ]. Thus, molecular mechanisms of de novo protein synthesis for memory consolidation may be shared by different learning paradigms. Similarly to the consolidation of courtship LTM, its maintenance also requires CrebB-mediated transcription [ 9 ]. However, the molecular mechanisms of courtship LTM maintenance still remain largely elusive.
Drosophila Apterous (Ap) is one of the most well-studied LIM homeodomain (LIM-HD) proteins. Similar to other LIM-HD proteins, Ap has 2 protein–protein interaction domains (LIM) and a DNA-binding homeodomain motif (HD). Ap coordinates with its cofactor Chip (Chi), and the multimeric Ap/Chi complex (hereafter referred to as Ap/Chi) acts as a positive regulator of transcription [ 17 ]. In Drosophila, ap was initially identified as a regulatory gene in wing development [ 18 , 19 ]. The subsequent investigations revealed that Ap and Chi are also essential for nervous system development [ 17 , 20 – 23 ]. Since Ap continues to be expressed in many neurons in the adult fly brain, it may have brain functions other than the regulation of neural development. In fact, Ap in the adult brain neurons expressing a neuropeptide Pdf is required for proper sleep/wake regulation [ 24 ]. Furthermore, Aranha and colleagues have reported that ap is expressed in MB neurons using an ap enhancer trap GAL4 line previously shown to accurately report ap expression [ 25 ]. Therefore, Ap in the MBs may play a role in Drosophila learning and memory. However, functions of Ap in MBs remain unclarified.
Previously, we and others reported the unexpected finding that the Drosophila circadian clock gene period (per) plays a vital role in memory consolidation induced by courtship conditioning and aversive olfactory classical conditioning [ 4 , 11 , 12 ]. This finding suggests that per-expressing clock neurons are also essential for LTM. In Drosophila, clock neurons expressing the neuropeptide pigment dispersing factor (Pdf; hereafter referred to as Pdf neurons) are critically involved in generating circadian rhythms. Pdf neurons consist of 2 neural clusters, small ventral–lateral neurons (s-LNvs) and large ventral–lateral neurons (l-LNvs) [ 13 ]. s-LNvs are believed to act as circadian pacemaker neurons [ 13 , 14 ], and l-LNvs are essential for sleep/wake regulation [ 15 , 16 ]. We have recently found that Pdf in l-LNvs also regulates the maintenance of LTM induced by courtship conditioning (hereafter referred to as courtship LTM) [ 9 ]. Thus, intercellular communication from l-LNvs to MB neurons is likely to play a crucial role in courtship LTM maintenance. However, little is known about whether Pdf neurons are also involved in other memory processes (e.g., consolidation and recall of LTM) and how Pdf neurons modulate courtship LTM formed and maintained in MBs.
A newly formed labile memory in the brain is consolidated into a more stable long-term memory (LTM), which is maintained until recall. The fruitfly Drosophila melanogaster is an excellent model organism for investigating the molecular mechanisms of learning and memory because it can efficiently learn and remember in various contexts, and advanced genetics is available for identifying and characterizing genes relevant to different aspects of memory processes [ 1 – 3 ]. Regardless of the learning paradigm, cAMP response element binding protein (CREB)-mediated transcription is essential for memory consolidation to establish LTM in Drosophila [ 4 – 6 ]. Many genes involved in memory consolidation have been identified, and its underlying molecular mechanisms are becoming elucidated [ 7 , 8 ]. LTM maintenance also requires the transcriptional activation of CREB in Drosophila [ 9 , 10 ], suggesting that de novo protein synthesis via transcription is also necessary for LTM maintenance. Mushroom bodies (MBs), a higher-order brain center for learning and memory [ 1 – 3 ], play critical roles in both the consolidation and maintenance of LTM [ 9 , 10 ]. However, despite recent progress, the molecular and cellular mechanisms underlying the consolidation and maintenance of LTM remain incompletely understood.
Since increased responses to GABA in MB α lobes were also detected in ap null /+ ( S8 Fig ), it is possible that the overresponse to GABA in MB α/β neurons is the cause of the impairment of 5-day memory in ap null /+ flies. However, unlike the Pdf neurons, the overexpression of Rdl in MB α/β neurons driven by R41C10 did not affect 5-day memory ( S8C Fig ).
In Pdf neurons, ap knockdown impaired 1-day memory after 7-hour conditioning ( Fig 4 ), while Chi knockdown did not ( Fig 6E ). Similarly, increased responses to GABA in l-LNvs were detected in ap null /+ but not in Chi null /+. Thus, the overresponses to GABA in l-LNvs may cause 1-day memory impairment in ap null /+ flies. If the reduced Ap expression in ap null /+ leads to a decrease in l-LNv excitability due to augmented responses to the inhibitory neurotransmitter GABA and the exaggerated inhibition of LNv excitability causes the 1-day memory impairment, the 1-day memory phenotype in heterozygous ap null flies may be compensated for by the reduced Rdl expression in l-LNvs. As we expected, the knockdown of Rdl in Pdf neurons compensated for the impaired 1-day memory in ap null /+ flies ( Fig 9E ). Furthermore, the Pdf neuron–specific overexpression of Rdl in WT background induced 1-day memory impairment ( Fig 9F ). This result is consistent with the aversive effect of the electrical silencing of Pdf neurons on LTM ( Fig 8 ).
The underlying data can be found in S1 Data . (A–D) Ex vivo imaging using SuperClomeleon. (A and B) UAS-SuperClomeleon/R61G12 flies were used. PTX, picrotoxin. (A) Traces of mean SuperClomeleon response to 400 μM GABA and 1 μM TTX with or without bath-applied 100 μM PTX. Error bars, SE; N = 12 in each trace. (B) Maximum percentage change in fluorescence of SuperClomeleon related to (A). Student t test was used. (C) UAS-SuperClomeleon/R61G12 flies (WT), UAS-SuperClomeleon/ap null ; R61G12/+ (ap nill /+), and UAS-SuperClomeleon/Chi null ; R61G12/+ (Chi null /+) flies were used. Traces of mean SuperClomeleon response to 400 μM GABA and 1 μM TTX. Error bars, SE; N = 20 to 24 in each trace. (D) Maximum percentage change in fluorescence of SuperClomeleon related to (C). Nonparametric ANOVA (Kruskal–Wallis test) followed by post hoc analysis using the Steel–Dwass test was carried out for multiple comparisons. (E and F) One-day memory after 7-hour conditioning. ***, P < 0.001; NS, not significant. (E) Rdl knockdown in Pdf neurons in ap null /+. ap null /+, UAS-Rdl RNAi/R61G12, and ap null /UAS-Rdl RNAi; R61G12 flies were used. (F) Rdl overexpression in Pdf neurons. Pdf-GAL4/+, UAS-Rdl/+, and Pdf-GAL4/UAS-Rdl flies were used. (B, D, E, and F) Box plots show fifth, 25th, 75th, and 95th centiles. In the box plots, the black square in each box indicates the mean, the line in each box is drawn at the median. Ap, Apterous; GABA, gamma-aminobutyric acid; l-LNv, large ventral–lateral clock neuron; Pdf, pigment dispersing factor; RNAi, RNA interference; WT, wild-type.
To visualize the response to GABA in l-LNvs, the FRET-based Cl − probe, SuperClomeleon, was used [ 44 , 45 ]. First, in flies with WT ap, we confirmed robust Cl − responses to 400 μM GABA in l-LNvs in the presence of TTX, while the responses were blocked by the GABA A R antagonist picrotoxin ( Fig 9A and 9B ). Similar Cl − responses were observed in heterozygous Chi null flies ( Fig 9C and 9D ). Compared with WT and heterozygous Chi null flies, heterozygous ap null flies showed robust increases in Cl − responses ( Fig 9C and 9D ), indicating that the response to GABA in l-LNvs is augmented when the ap function is suppressed. Since Rdl is also expressed in MB neurons [ 46 ], we investigated whether Ap in MB α/β neurons is also involved in the response to GABA. As was observed in l-LNvs, Cl − responses in MB α lobes in heterozygous ap null flies, but not in heterozygous Chi null flies, were greater than those in WT flies ( S8A and S8B Fig ). These results indicate that Ap is necessary for appropriate Cl − responses to GABA in a Chi-independent manner.
Our results show that Ap in Pdf neurons is necessary for memory consolidation in a Chi-independent manner (Figs 3 , 5 , and 6 ). Since both the disruption of neurotransmission and the electrical silencing mimic the effect of Ap loss of function and impair memory consolidation, ap mutations or knockdown may reduce the excitability of Pdf neurons. A Drosophila ionotropic GABA A receptor (GABA A R), Resistant to Dieldrin (Rdl), is expressed in Pdf neurons. Anti-RDL antibody staining reveals that RDL is highly expressed in l-LNv somata, while little or no expression is observed in s-LNv somata [ 15 , 43 ]. Furthermore, electrophysiological analysis revealed that l-LNvs respond to GABA [ 43 ]. Considering the physiological properties of l-LNvs, we next investigated the possibility that Ap in l-LNvs is involved in the response to GABA.
(A–D) Five-day memory after 7-hour conditioning. Pdf-GAL4/+ (control) and Pdf-GAL4/UAS-shi ts1 flies were used. *, P < 0.05; **, P < 0.01; NS, not significant. (A) All experiments were performed at PT (25°C). (B) Flies were kept at RT during conditioning. (C) Flies were kept at RT for 48 to 72 hours after 7-hour conditioning. (D) Flies were kept at RT for 12 hours before the test. (E) Five-day memory after 7-hour conditioning. Flies were kept at RT for 24 hours before the end of conditioning. UAS-Kir2.1/Pdf-GAL4; tub-GAL80 ts /+ flies were used. UAS-Kir2.1/tub-GAL80 ts flies were used as the control. (A–E) Box plots for a set of CI data show fifth, 25th, 75th, and 95th centiles. In the box plots, the black square in each box indicates the mean, the line in each box is drawn at the median, the white boxes indicate naive males, and the gray boxes indicate conditioned males. The underlying data can be found in S1 Data . CI, courtship index; MI, memory index; N, sample size; U, Mann–Whitney U; P, probability. Pdf, pigment dispersing factor; PT, permissive temperature; RT, restrictive temperature.
To determine whether neurotransmission from Pdf neurons is necessary for proper memory consolidation, we used the temperature-sensitive Dynamin mutation shibire ts1 (shi ts1 ). The targeted expression of shi ts1 can inhibit neurotransmission in a spatially specific and temperature-dependent manner [ 42 ]. Pdf-GAL4/UAS-shi ts1 flies showed LTM at PT ( Fig 8A ). We found that the disruption of neurotransmission during the conditioning phase impaired LTM ( Fig 8B ), but not during the memory maintenance or test phase ( Fig 8C and 8D ). Thus, neurotransmission from Pdf neurons is necessary for proper memory consolidation. In addition, we performed the electrical silencing of Pdf neurons using the inwardly rectifying Kir2.1 channel combined with the TARGET system. For the silencing of Pdf neurons during memory consolidation, we shifted the temperature from PT to RT and vice versa during the conditioning phase. As was observed in the disruption of neurotransmission using shi ts1 , the electrical silencing of Pdf neurons during conditioning impaired LTM ( Fig 8E ).
(A) All experiments were performed at PT (25°C). (B) Flies were kept at RT for 24 hours before the end of conditioning. (C) Flies were kept at RT for 48 to 72 hours after 7-hour conditioning. (A–C) Five-day memory after 7-hour conditioning. UAS-Chi RNAi (VDRC)/+; tub-GAL80 ts /R41C10 and control (UAS-Chi RNAi (VDRC)/tub-GAL80 ts ) flies were used. Box plots for a set of CI data show fifth, 25th, 75th, and 95th centiles. In the box plots, the black square in each box indicates the mean, the line in each box is drawn at the median, the white boxes indicate naive males, and the gray boxes indicate conditioned males. The underlying data can be found in S1 Data . CI, courtship index; MI, memory index; N, sample size; U, Mann–Whitney U; P, probability; **, P < 0.01; NS, not significant. Chi, Chip; LTM, long-term memory; MB, mushroom body; PT, permissive temperature; RNAi, RNA interference; RT, restrictive temperature.
To verify that Chi is required in neurons for 5-day memory, nSyb-GAL4/UAS-Chi RNAi (VDRC) and nSyb-GAL4/UAS-Chi RNAi (TRiP) flies were used. We found that the pan-neuronal knockdown of Chi with 2 independent UAS-Chi RNAi lines both impaired 5-day memory ( S4B Fig ). In the following experiments, UAS-Chi RNAi (VDRC) was used. We confirmed that Chi knockdown in MB α/β neurons induces 5-day memory impairment ( Fig 6E ; Permutation test, R41C10/UAS-Chi RNAi versus UAS control, P = 0.007, R41C10/UAS-Chi RNAi versus GAL4 control, P = 0.015). However, Chi knockdown in Pdf neurons had no effect on LTM ( Fig 6E ). Next, we temporarily knocked down Chi in MB α/β neurons to examine when Chi is required in these neurons during memory processing for LTM. LTM was impaired when Chi was knocked down during the memory maintenance phase but not during the conditioning phase ( Fig 7C ; Permutation test; P = 0.002). Thus, we concluded that Chi in MB α/β neurons contributes to memory maintenance rather than LTM consolidation. Taken together, these findings support the idea that Ap cooperates with Chi in MB α/β neurons to maintain LTM, but plays a role in l-LNvs to consolidate memory independently of Chi.
The underlying data can be found in S1 Data . (A) One-day memory after 7-hour conditioning. WT and Chi null /+ flies were used. NS, not significant. (B) Real-time qRT-PCR analysis of ap mRNA expression level. nSyb-GAL4 was used for the induction of Chi RNAi (VDRC). Mean ± SEM was calculated from 5 replicates. One-way ANOVA followed by post hoc analysis using Scheffe test for multiple comparisons was used. Bars with the same letter indicate values that are not significantly different (P > 0.05). (C) Pdf neuron–specific Chi knockdown in Chi null /+ flies. One-day memory after 7-hour conditioning. UAS-Chi RNAi/+; R61G12/+ (control) and UAS-Chi RNAi/Chi null ; R61G12/+ flies were used. (D) Five-day memory after 7-hour conditioning. WT and Chi null /+ flies were used. *, P < 0.05. (A, C, and D) Box plots for a set of CI data show fifth, 25th, 75th, and 95th centiles. In the box plots, the black square in each box indicates the mean, the line in each box is drawn at the median, the white boxes indicate naive males, and the gray boxes indicate conditioned males. CI, courtship index; MI, memory index; N, sample size; U, Mann–Whitney U; P, probability. (E) Chi knockdown using Pdf-GAL4 and R41C10. Five-day memory after 7-hour conditioning. Box plots for a set of CI data show 10th, 25th, 75th, and 90th centiles. In the box plots, the black square in each box indicates the mean, the line in each box is drawn at the median, the white boxes indicate naive males, and the gray boxes indicate conditioned males. CI, courtship index; MI, memory index; N, sample size; U, Mann–Whitney U; P, probability; *, P < 0.05; **, P < 0.01; NS, not significant. Chi, Chip; LTM, long-term memory; MB, mushroom body; qRT-PCR, quantitative reverse transcription PCR; RNAi, RNA interference; WT, wild-type.
Since the transcriptional activity of Ap requires the formation of a protein complex with a cofactor, Chi [ 23 , 40 ], we reasoned that Chi is also involved in LTM. To investigate this possibility, a null allele Chi e5.5 (hereafter referred to as Chi null ) was used [ 41 ]. Since Chi null is also homozygous lethal as was observed in ap null , we used heterozygous mutant flies (Chi null /+). Unlike the LTM phenotype in ap null /+, 1-day memory after 7-hour conditioning was not impaired in Chi null /+ flies ( Fig 6A ; Permutation test, P = 0.087). Since a 50% reduction of Chi function in Chi null /+ flies may be insufficient to induce LTM impairment, we further knocked down Chi in Pdf neurons in Chi null /+ flies and examined the effect of this knockdown on LTM. The effectiveness of Chi RNAi (VDRC) was confirmed by qRT-PCR using a pan-neuronal GAL4 line (nSyb-GAL4); Chi RNA levels were reduced by about 55% ( Fig 6B ; Scheffe’s multiple comparisons, GAL4 control versus F 1 , P < 0.001, UAS control versus F 1 , P < 0.001). Even when Chi was knocked down in a Pdf neuron–specific manner in Chi null /+ background flies, 1-day memory was not affected ( Fig 6C ; Permutation test, P = 0.488). In contrast to 1-day memory, 5-day memory in Chi null /+ flies was impaired ( Fig 6D , Permutation test, P = 0.026), indicating that Chi is involved in LTM maintenance rather than in memory consolidation.
(A, B, and D) ap null /UAS-ap flies were used as a control. Box plots for a set of CI data show 5th, 25th, 75th, and 95th centiles. In the box plots, the black square in each box indicates the mean, the line in each box is drawn at the median, the white boxes indicate naive males, and the gray boxes indicate conditioned males. The underlying data can be found in S1 Data . CI, courtship index; MI, memory index; N, sample size; U, Mann–Whitney U; P, probability; *, P < 0.05; ***, P < 0.001; NS, not significant. (A) One-day memory after 7-hour conditioning. (B) Two-day memory after 7-hour conditioning. (C) Stacked confocal image showing an anterior view of the adult brain. The scale bar represents 100 μm. Pdf-GAL4/UAS-mCD8::GFP; R41C10/+ flies were used. (D) Five-day memory after 7-hour conditioning. Ap, Apterous; LTM, long-term memory; MB, mushroom body; Pdf, pigment dispersing factor.
Since cell type–specific ap knockdown experiments revealed that sufficient ap function is required for normal LTM, we next examined whether ap expression in MB α/β and/or Pdf neurons can rescues the LTM phenotype in ap null /+ flies. If 1-day memory impairment after 7-hour conditioning in ap null /+ flies is due to defective memory consolidation resulting from the reduced function of Ap in Pdf neurons, the ability of ap null /+ flies to show 1-day memory should be restored by expressing WT ap in Pdf neurons. As shown in Fig 5A , the targeted expression of ap in Pdf neurons rescued the impairment of 1-day memory in ap null /+ flies ( Fig 5A ; Permutation test; P < 0.001). However, the targeted expression of ap in MB α/β neurons did not rescue the impairment of 1-day memory in ap null /+ flies ( Fig 5A ; Permutation test; P = 0.130). Furthermore, Pdf neuron–specific ap expression did not rescue impairment of memory on day 2 after 7-hour conditioning in ap null /+ flies ( Fig 5B , 2-day memory; Permutation test, P = 0.496). These findings indicate that Pdf neuron–specific Ap expression in ap null /+ flies is insufficient to maintain LTM for 2 days. For 5-day memory, the targeted expression of ap in either Pdf or MB α/β neurons was not sufficient to rescue the ap null /+ phenotype ( Fig 5D ; Permutation test, versus Pdf-GAL4, P = 0.778, versus R41C10, P = 0.259). However, when ap was expressed in both Pdf and MB α/β neurons using Pdf-GAL4; R41C10 flies ( Fig 5C ), LTM impairment in ap null /+ flies was rescued ( Fig 5D ; Permutation test; P = 0.042). These findings support the idea that Ap in Pdf neurons is responsible for memory consolidation, whereas that in MB α/β neurons is responsible for LTM maintenance.
Next, we examined when ap in Pdf neurons is required during processing for LTM. In contrast to temporal knockdown in MB α/β neurons, LTM was impaired when Ap was knocked down during conditioning but not during maintenance ( Fig 3B ; Permutation test; P = 0.019). These findings indicate that Ap in Pdf neurons mainly contributes to memory consolidation rather than LTM maintenance.
(A) Five-day memory after 7-hour conditioning. ap RNAi was driven by Pdf-GAL4, c929, and R18F07. Box plots for a set of CI data show 10th, 25th, 75th, and 90th centiles. In the box plots, the black square in each box indicates the mean, the line in each box is drawn at the median, the white boxes indicate naive males, and the gray boxes indicate conditioned males. CI, courtship index; MI, memory index; N, sample size; U, Mann–Whitney U; P, probability; **, P < 0.01; ***, P < 0.001; NS, not significant. (B) Five-day memory after 7-hour conditioning. ap RNAi was driven by R14F03. *, P < 0.05; **, P < 0.01; NS, not significant. (C) l-LNv–specific expression of mCD8::GFP. c929/R61G12-LexA; LexAop-FLPL/UAS>STOP>mCD8::GFP flies were used. Brains were stained with an anti-Pdf antibody and an anti-GFP antibody. Magenta, Pdf; green, mCD8::GFP. Confocal section images at the level of Pdf neurons of the adult brain. Scale bars represent 10 μm. (D) l-LNv–specific knockdown of ap. Five-day memory after 7-hour conditioning. c929/R61G12-LexA; LexAop-FLPL/UAS>STOP>ap RNAi flies were used. **, P < 0.01; NS, not significant. (B and D) Box plots for a set of CI data show fifth, 25th, 75th, and 95th centiles. In the box plots, the black square in each box indicates the mean, the line in each box is drawn at the median, the white boxes indicate naive males, and the gray boxes indicate conditioned males. CI, courtship index; MI, memory index; N, sample size; U, Mann–Whitney U; P, probability. (A, B, and D). The underlying data can be found in S1 Data . Ap, Apterous; l-LNv, large ventral–lateral clock neuron; LTM, long-term memory; Pdf, pigment dispersing factor; RNAi, RNA interference.
Ap knockdown by hs-GAL4 shows that Ap is involved in both memory consolidation and maintenance ( Fig 1 ). Since Ap in MB α/β neurons mainly contributed to LTM maintenance but not to consolidation ( Fig 3C ), it is likely that brain neurons other than MB α/β neurons are involved in Ap-dependent memory consolidation. We previously identified that Ap is expressed in Pdf neurons consisting of s-LNvs and l-LNvs [ 24 ]. Thus, we next examined whether ap knockdown in Pdf neurons affects LTM. First, the following 3 GAL4 lines were used: (1) Pdf-GAL4, which drives GAL4 expression in both s-LNvs and l-LNvs; (2) c929, which drives GAL4 expression in peptidergic neurons including l-LNvs [ 36 ]; and (3) R18F07, which drives GAL4 expression in s-LNvs and only weakly in one of the l-LNvs [ 9 ]. When Pdf-GAL4 and c929 were used to knock down ap, LTM was impaired ( Fig 4A ; Permutation test; in Pdf-GAL4/UAS-ap RNAi, versus UAS control, P < 0.001, versus GAL4 control, P < 0.001; in c929/UAS-ap RNAi, versus UAS control, P < 0.001, versus GAL4 control, P = 0.001), whereas ap knockdown by R18F07 did not impair LTM ( Fig 4A ; in R18F07/UAS-ap RNAi, versus UAS control, P = 0.099, versus GAL4 control, P = 0.916). These results suggest that Ap in l-LNvs is necessary for LTM. Since GAL4 expression in c929 is detected in the neuronal subset of the abdominal ganglion [ 37 ] and Pdf neuropeptide is also expressed in the abdominal ganglion [ 38 ], Ap in the abdominal ganglion may be necessary for LTM. Thus, we next used a GAL4 line, R14F03, which drives GAL4 expression in l-LNvs but not in the abdominal ganglion (
https://flweb.janelia.org/cgi-bin/view_flew_imagery.cgi?line=R14F03 ). If Ap in the abdominal ganglion is essential for LTM, ap knockdown using R14F03 should not affect LTM. However, R14F03/UAS-ap RNAi flies showed LTM impairment ( Fig 4B ), indicating that Ap in l-LNvs is necessary for LTM. Next, to determine whether Ap in l-LNvs but not in other brain neurons is essential for LTM, we performed l-LNv–specific Ap knockdown using the Flp-out system [ 39 ] with c929 and R61G12-LexA. In R61G12-LexA, LexA is expressed in l-LNvs and s-LNvs [ 9 ]. Using this system, we can knock down targeted genes specifically in the c929 and R61G12-LexA-coexpressing neurons. The effectiveness of the Flp-out system was confirmed using the GFP reporter. In control flies carrying c929, R61G12-LexA, LexAop-FLPL, and UAS>STOP> GFP, the GFP signal was detected in all l-LNvs but not in s-LNvs ( Fig 4C ). As shown in Fig 4D , the Flip-out experiments with UAS>STOP>ap RNAi demonstrated that l-LNv–specific ap knockdown impaired LTM (Permutation test, P = 0.002). This result also supports the conclusion that Ap in l-LNvs is indispensable for LTM.
(A) All experiments were performed at PT (25°C). (B) Flies were kept at RT for 24 hours before the end of conditioning. (C) Flies were kept at RT for 48 to 72 hours after 7-hour conditioning. (A–C) Memory on day 5 in UAS-ap RNAi/+; tub-GAL80 ts /R41C10 and control (UAS-ap RNAi/tub-GAL80 ts ) flies. Box plots for a set of CI data show fifth, 25th, 75th, and 95th centiles. In the box plots, the black square in each box indicates the mean, the line in each box is drawn at the median, the white boxes indicate naive males, and the gray boxes indicate conditioned males. The underlying data can be found in S1 Data . CI, courtship index; MI, memory index; N, sample size; U, Mann–Whitney U; P, probability; *, P < 0.05; **, P < 0.01; NS, not significant. Ap, Apterous; LTM, long-term memory; MB, mushroom body; Pdf, pigment dispersing factor; PT, permissive temperature; RNAi, RNA interference; RT, restrictive temperature.
To further explore whether Ap in MB α/β neurons is responsible for memory consolidation or maintenance, we temporarily knocked down ap in MB α/β neurons using the TARGET system [ 35 ]. To knock down ap during the memory consolidation or maintenance phase, the temperature was increased to 30°C for 24 hours during 2 different experimental periods: (1) starting at 24 hours before the end of conditioning (conditioning phase) [ Fig 3B , restrictive temperature (RT) (1)]; and (2) starting at 48 hours after the end of conditioning (memory maintenance phase) [ Fig 3C , RT(2)]. When flies were kept at a permissive temperature (PT), they showed normal LTM regardless of their genotypes ( Fig 3A ). On the other hand, LTM was impaired when Ap was knocked down during the memory maintenance phase ( Fig 3C ), although it was intact when Ap was knocked down during the conditioning phase ( Fig 3B ). Thus, Ap in MB α/β neurons mainly contributes to LTM maintenance rather than memory consolidation.
To clarify whether ap mutations induce structural defects in the MB, we used ap GAL4 flies. Flies homozygous for ap GAL4 were lethal, as was previously reported [ 20 ]. However, we were able to obtaine escapers of the F 1 hybrid of ap GAL4 and ap rk568 flies (ap GAL4 /ap rk568 ) in which the ap function is severely reduced. We confirmed that they exhibit obvious structural defects of the MB with the α lobes branched into 2 regions ( S7A Fig , arrows). By contrast, no such structural defects of the MB were identified in ap GAL4 /+ ( S6A and S7B Figs). These results are consistent with the previous report indicating that escapers of the F 1 hybrid of ap GAL4 and ap P44 (a null allele) show severe morphological defects such as abnormal wings and an embryonic VNC structure, but that ap GAL4 /+ flies show no such abnormalities [ 20 ]. Furthermore, in ap GAL4 /+ flies as well as ap null /+ and ap rk568 /+ flies, 1-hour memory after 1-hour conditioning (STM) was intact, whereas 5-day memory after 7-hour conditioning (LTM) was defective ( S6C and S6D Fig ). In summary, flies heterozygous for ap mutant alleles did not show obvious structural defects in the MB, and STM was intact. Nonetheless, they showed LTM impairment.
We next examined whether Ap-expressing neurons are present in MBs. To visualize Ap-expressing neurons, we first used an ap mutant allele ap md544 with a P-element (pGawB) insertion (hereafter referred to as ap GAL4 ). ap GAL4 was previously shown to accurately report the expression of ap [ 33 ]. Flies homozygous for ap GAL4 were lethal, but the heterozygous flies were viable [ 20 ]. In ap GAL4 /UAS-mCD8::GFP flies, GFP signals, which colocalized with Fas II, a marker of MB lobes, were primarily detected in a subset of MB αβ neurons but not in MB γ neurons or α’/β’ neurons ( S6A Fig ). Furthermore, they were abolished by MB-GAL80 ( S6B Fig ), indicating that ap GAL4 signals indeed came from the MB neurons. Next, we used ap::GFP knock-in flies, which express a GFP reporter in a pattern consistent with endogenous Ap expression [ 34 ]. Ap::GFP and the nucleus-targeted mCherry reporter for MB α/β neurons were colocalized in many neurons ( Fig 2B ), and only a few neurons showed colocalization in MB γ or α’/β’ neurons ( Fig 2C and 2D ). Combined, these results support the conclusion that Ap is predominantly expressed in MB αβ neurons.
(A) Five-day memory after 7-hour conditioning. ap RNAi was driven by MB α/β -GAL4 (R41C10), MB γ-GAL4 (R55D03), and MB α’/β’-GAL4 (c305a). Box plots for a set of CI data show 10th, 25th, 75th, and 90th centiles. In the box plots, the black square in each box indicates the mean, the line in each box is drawn at the median, the white boxes indicate naive males, and the gray boxes indicate conditioned males. The underlying data can be found in S1 Data . CI, courtship index; MI, memory index; N, sample size; U, Mann–Whitney U; P, probability; *, P < 0.05; NS, not significant. (B–D) Ap-expressing neurons in MBs. Confocal sectional images at the level of Kenyon cells in MBs. Scale bars, 50 μm; green, Ap::GFP; magenta, mCherry::NLS driven by MB-GAL4 lines. Dotted lines and arrows show the colocalization of Ap::GFP and mCherry::NLS. (B) ap::GFP/+; UAS-mCherry::NLS/R41C10 flies were used. (C) ap::GFP/+; UAS-mCherry::NLS/R55D03 flies were used. (D) ap::GFP/c305a; UAS-mCherry::NLS/+ flies were used. Ap, Apterous; LTM, long-term memory; MB, mushroom body; RNAi, RNA interference.
To examine the significance of Ap in the nervous system for memory consolidation and/or maintenance, we knocked down ap in neurons using UAS-ap RNAi in combination with several GAL4 lines. When a pan-neural nSyb-GAL4 line was used, 2 independent UAS-ap RNAi transgenes (NIG-fly and TRiP) both resulted in impaired LTM ( S4A Fig ), indicating that Ap in the nervous system is essential for LTM. In the following experiments, UAS-ap RNAi (NIG-fly) was used. To examine whether Ap in the Drosophila memory center is involved in LTM, we used 2 MB-GAL4 lines, OK107 and 30Y. In both lines, GAL4 is expressed in all MB lobes [ 32 ]. ap knockdown using these MB-GAL4 lines impaired LTM ( S5B Fig ). Furthermore, to define the particular MB neurons critical for courtship LTM, 3 GAL4 lines (R41C10, R55D03, and c305a) driving GAL4 expression in different neuronal subsets of MBs were used [ 9 ]. LTM was intact when Ap was knocked down in MB γ or α’/β’ neurons, whereas Ap knockdown in MB α/β neurons impaired LTM ( Fig 2A , MI; R41C10/+ versus R41C10 / UAS-ap RNAi, Permutation test, P = 0.011; UAS-ap RNAi/+ versus R41C10 / UAS-ap RNAi, Permutation test; P = 0.033). In R41C10, GAL4 expression is controlled by a 3,500-bp fragment from the intronic region of ap. Both Jenelia’s Flylight project (
https://flweb.janelia.org/cgi-bin/view_flew_imagery.cgi?line=R41C10 ) and our previous study [ 9 ] demonstrated that the reporter gene expression driven by R41C10 is primarily observed in MB αβ neurons. Although Jenelia’s Flylight project detected signals in optic lobes and visual projection neurons, the expression outside the MB αβ neurons is very weak and limited. Thus, the result with R41C10 indicates that MB αβ neurons are most likely responsible for Ap-dependent courtship LTM.
Since Ap plays essential roles in neurodevelopmental events in the central nervous system [ 17 ], it is possible that the observed LTM impairment is caused by neurodevelopmental defects in ap mutant flies. To determine if the ap expression in adulthood is critical for LTM, we examined the effects of temporal knockdown of ap expression on the LTM phenotype using flies heterozygous for heat-shock (hs)-GAL4 and UAS-ap RNA interference (RNAi) lines (hs-GAL4/UAS-ap RNAi). First, the effectiveness of ap RNAi was confirmed by quantitative reverse transcription PCR (qRT-PCR). Flies were heat-shocked (37°C) for 20 minutes, and total RNA was extracted 3 hours after heat-shock treatment. Heat-shocked hs-GAL4/UAS-ap RNAi flies showed about 50% reduction in ap expression level compared with control flies ( Fig 1C ; hs-GAL4/+, Student t test, t (8) = −1.474, P = 2.306; +/UAS-ap RNAi, U = 5, P = 0.117; hs-GAL4/UAS-ap RNAi, U = 4, P = 0.025). The ap expression level recovered to the non–heat-shocked level 48 hours after heat-shock treatment ( S2 Fig ). In the absence of heat-shock treatment, LTM in hs-GAL4/UAS-ap RNAi flies was detected [ Fig 1D–1F ; CI in HS− (1), U = 240, P = 0.002; CI in HS− (2), U = 206.5, P = 0.002; CI in HS− (3), U = 220, P = 0.004]. By contrast, LTM impairment was observed when flies were heat-shocked 3 hours before or immediately after 7-hour conditioning ( Fig 1D and 1E ; HS− versus HS+(1), Permutation test; P = 0.037; HS− versus HS+(2), Permutation test; P = 0.007). The impairment is due to the conditional knockdown of ap because heat-shock treatment did not affect LTM in GAL4 and UAS control flies (hs-GAL4/+ and UAS-ap RNAi/+ flies; S3 Fig ). These results suggest that normal Ap function is required for the consolidation and maintenance of LTM. However, when flies were heat-shocked 3 hours before the test, LTM was intact ( Fig 1F ; HS−(3) versus HS+(3), Permutation test; P = 0.956). On the basis of the results of the qRT-PCR analysis ( Fig 1C ), the ap expression level must be reduced to approximately 50% of the control level during the test. Therefore, reduced Ap function does not have a critical effect on LTM retrieval. Under this condition, the timing of the initiation of heat-shock treatment is considered to be within the memory maintenance phase. However, the treatment did not affect LTM. Therefore, the reduction in Ap expression level and the duration of reduced Ap expression (≤3 hours) induced by the treatment may be insufficient to inhibit the LTM maintenance.
On day 5 after 7-hour conditioning (hereafter referred to as 5-day memory), WT flies also showed a significant reduction in CI in a conditioning-dependent manner ( Fig 1B , CI). However, no significant differences in CI were detected between naive and conditioned ap rk568 /+ and ap null /+ ( Fig 1B , CI), and their LTM was impaired ( Fig 1B , MI; WT versus ap rk568 /+, Permutation test; P = 0.004; WT versus ap null /+, Permutation test; P = 0.015), indicating that Ap is essential for LTM specifically.
The underlying data can be found in S1 Data . (A) WT, ap rk568 /+, and ap null /+ flies were used in the experiments. Males were tested for 1 hour after 1-hour conditioning (1-hour memory). (B) WT, ap rk568 /+, and ap null /+ flies were used in the experiments. Males were tested on day 5 after 7-hour conditioning (5-day memory). (C) Real-time qRT-PCR analysis of ap mRNA expression level. hs-GAL4 was used for the induction of ap RNAi. Mean ± SEM was calculated from 5 to 6 replicates. HS−, non–heat-shocked flies. HS+, flies with heat-shock treatment (20 minutes) 3 hours before RNA extraction. *, P < 0.05; NS, not significant. Error bars show SEM in each figure. (D) Five-day memory after 7-hour conditioning in flies with or without a 20-minute heat-shock treatment 3 hours before conditioning [HS+ (1)]. (E) Five-day memory after 7-hour conditioning. HS+ (2), flies with heat-shock treatment immediately after conditioning. (F) Five-day memory after 7-hour conditioning. HS+ (3), flies with heat-shock treatment 3 hours before test. (A, B, and D–F) Box plots for a set of CI data show fifth, 25th, 75th, and 95th centiles. In the box plots, the black square in each box indicates the mean, the line in each box is drawn at the median, the white boxes indicate naive males, and the gray boxes indicate conditioned males. CI, courtship index; MI, memory index; N, sample size; U, Mann–Whitney U; P, probability; *, P < 0.05; **, P < 0.01; NS, not significant. (D–F), hs-GAL4/UAS-ap RNAi flies were used. Ap, Apterous; LTM, long-term memory; qRT-PCR, quantitative reverse transcription PCR; RNAi, RNA interference; WT, wild-type.
We examined whether Ap is involved in Drosophila memory using a courtship conditioning assay. In this assay, 1-hour conditioning generates short-term memory (STM), and 7-hour conditioning induces LTM [ 4 , 30 , 31 ]. STM persists for at least 1 hour, whereas LTM persists for at least 5 days [ 4 , 31 ]. Two mutant alleles of ap (ap rk568 and ap UGO35 ) were tested for the effects of ap mutations on LTM. ap rk568 is a strong hypomorphic allele with a P element insertion into the putative 5′-flanking region [ 18 ]. ap UGO35 (hereafter referred to as ap null ) is a null allele generated by the imprecise excision of the P element associated with ap rk568 [ 18 ]. Since ap rk568 and ap null are homozygous lethal, we used heterozygous mutants (ap rk568 /+ and ap null /+). In wild-type (WT), ap rk568 /+, and ap null /+ flies, the courtship index (CI), an indicator of male courtship activity, of conditioned males was significantly lower than that of naive males 1 hour after 1-hour conditioning ( Fig 1A , CI). To quantify courtship memory, memory index (MI) was calculated (refer to Materials and Methods ). No significant differences in MI were detected among 3 genotypes ( Fig 1A , MI; WT versusap rk568 /+, Permutation test; P = 0.550; WT versus ap null /+, Permutation test; P = 0.896). Thus, in ap rk568 /+ and ap null /+, 1-hour memory was intact. Furthermore, we examined whether male courtship suppression is detected 8 hours and 24 hours after 1-hour conditioning. The courtship suppression of conditioned males was evident in WT and ap rk568 /+ flies 8 hours after 1-hour conditioning ( S1 Fig ), whereas it was not detected 24 hours after 1-hour conditioning. These results demonstrate that courtship STM is maintained for at least 8 hours and that the ability of heterozygous ap rk568 flies to learn, form, and maintain STM is comparable to that of WT flies.
Discussion
The evolutionarily conserved LIM-HD protein Ap acts as a transcriptional activator, and it is essential for various developmental events in Drosophila [18,19,21,23]. However, the functions of Ap in mature adults have remained largely unknown. In this study, we found that Ap in the l-LNvs and MB α/β neurons in the adult brain plays an acute physiological role in the consolidation and maintenance of LTM induced by courtship conditioning. This conclusion was drawn mainly from the observation that the neuronal subset-specific knockdown of ap in adult flies results in the impairment of specific memory processes.
Although Ap is critical for normal development [17], including the development of the nervous system, we do not consider that the LTM defects in flies with ap loss of function are caused by developmental failure because no obvious structural defects in MB were identified in apGAL4/+ (S6A Fig) and apnull/+ (S7B Fig) flies. Furthermore, we previously reported that Pdf neuron–specific ap knockdown does not affect the number of Pdf-positive cells and varicosities [24]. Thus, heterozygous ap mutant or ap knockdown flies are unlikely to have significant developmental effects on the brain structure. On the other hand, we cannot completely rule out the possibility that a 50% reduction in Ap function induces minor developmental defects in brain neurons (e.g., abnormal neuronal projection and synaptogenesis). However, although the ap expression of MB neurons should be about 50% of that of the WT flies in apnull/Pdf-GAL4; UAS-ap/+ flies, 1-day memory after 7-hour conditioning was intact in these flies (Fig 5). Therefore, even if the heterozygous apnull mutation causes minor developmental defects in neural projection and synaptogenesis in MB neurons, these flies can consolidate memory and maintain it for 1 day. Furthermore, in the experiments on the temporal knockdown of ap, we revealed that adult-specific ap expression is essential for courtship memory consolidation and maintenance (Fig 3). Taken together, we concluded that centrally expressed Ap is critical for the acute physiological regulation of the consolidation and maintenance of courtship LTM.
We identified that Ap and its cofactor Chi in MB α/β neurons are indispensable for LTM maintenance. Ap LIM domains interact with the Chi LIM interaction domain, and Chi can homodimerize through the dimerization domain [17,23,40] (Fig 10A). Since Ap/Chi regulates the transcription of Ap target genes [17], it is most likely that Ap/Chi is necessary to alter gene expression profiles in MB α/β neurons so that proteins required for the maintenance of courtship LTM are appropriately provided at the correct time and place (Fig 10A). It is important to identify the changes in gene expression in MB α/β neurons that are dependent on courtship conditioning, and are critical for LTM maintenance. Future experiments using cell type–specific transcriptome analysis will help us address this important issue.
PPT PowerPoint slide
PNG larger image
TIFF original image Download: Fig 10. Possible model of Ap functions in the consolidation and maintenance of LTM. (A) The Ap/Chi complex induces gene expression. Subsequently, proteins required for maintaining consolidated LTM are produced in MB α/β neurons. (B) In l-LNvs, Ap may inhibit GABA responses via Rdl in a Chi-independent manner. (C) Intercellular communication from l-LNvs to MB neurons plays a crucial role in forming stable LTM. Non-MB neurons may mediate intercellular communication between l-LNvs and MB neurons. Ap, Apterous; Chi, Chip; GABA, gamma-aminobutyric acid; l-LNv, large ventral–lateral clock neuron; LTM, long-term memory; MB, mushroom body.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3001459.g010
In Drosophila wing development, it has been proposed that Beadex (Bx) [also known as Drosophila LIM-only protein (dLMO)] modulates the Ap function by interfering with the formation of Ap/Chi [47]. In this scenario, Bx can modify Ap-dependent transcription in the presence of Chi in the adult fly brain. In fact, Bx expression is prominent in several distinct regions of the adult brain including MBs and Pdf neurons [48]. Since the Ap/Chi complex is critical for LTM maintenance in MB α/β neurons, Bx may be involved in LTM maintenance by regulating the Ap/Chi complex activity. Interestingly, Bx in MB neurons is involved in the LTM maintenance induced by aversive olfactory conditioning [10], implying that the Ap/Chi complex plays an important role in LTM in different learning paradigms. It will be interesting to see whether Bx also affects the maintenance of courtship LTM via the modification of Ap/Chi-dependent transcription in MB α/β neurons.
Unlike the Chi-dependent role of Ap in MB α/β neurons, Ap in Pdf-positive l-LNvs was essential for memory consolidation to establish courtship LTM in a Chi-independent manner. Thus, the molecular mechanisms responsible for the Ap function in l-LNvs should be different from those in the previously reported model proposing that Ap forms a complex with Chi and regulates the transcription. For the Chi-independent Ap function, we provide evidence that Ap is necessary for appropriate Cl− responses to GABA in l-LNvs; reduced ap function enhanced the Cl− responses in l-LNvs. By contrast, reduced Chi function did not affect the Cl− response to GABA. Considering the Ap mutant phenotype, WT Ap likely contributes to reducing the responses of l-LNv to GABA and maintains their higher excitability. Consistent with the effect of the ap mutation on LTM consolidation and responses to GABA in l-LNv, we found that Rdl knockdown in Pdf neurons compensates for the defective 1-day memory observed in heterozygous ap null mutant flies. These results indicate that the ap-dependent and Chi-independent regulation of responses to GABA in l-LNv is critical for LTM consolidation. Although it remains elusive how Ap regulates the responses to GABA, our findings raise the possibility that Ap reduces Cl− influx by suppressing the expression of Rdl in l-LNvs or modulating the channel properties (e.g., receptor trafficking and localization) (Fig 10B). Further study will be needed to examine the factors that modify the responses to GABA owing to ap mutations in l-LNvs.
As was observed in l-LNvs (Fig 9), Ap also regulates responses to GABA in MB α lobes (S8 Fig), which are the critical neuronal subsets for Ap-dependent LTM maintenance. However, the modulation of GABA responses in MB neurons may not be essential for LTM maintenance because Rdl overexpression in MB α/β neurons has no effect on LTM (S8C Fig), although it induces LTM impairment in Pdf neurons. Furthermore, the knockdown of Chi in MB α/β neurons has no effect on GABA responses in MB α lobes (Fig 9) but severely disrupts LTM maintenance (Fig 7). Taken together, in R41C10-positive MB α/β neurons, the transcriptional activity of Ap/Chi rather than the modulation of GABA responses is considered to be involved in LTM maintenance.
As is well known in the olfactory associative learning paradigm, MB neurons are critically involved in memory consolidation to establish courtship LTM [9,28,31,49,50]. In this study, we identified that synaptic transmission from l-LNvs is necessary for memory consolidation (Fig 8), suggesting that intercellular communication from l-LNvs to MB neurons plays a crucial role in establishing stable LTM. Since the l-LNvs arborize in the most distal layer of the medulla and the accessory medullae (aMe), and they project to the other hemisphere through the posterior optic tract (POT) [51], it is unlikely that l-LNvs synaptically project and transmit to MB α/β neurons directly. Although the details of the specific neural circuits from l-LNvs to MBs are still unknown, non-MB interneurons likely mediate communication between l-LNvs and MB neurons (Fig 10C). The identification of neuronal circuits connecting l-LNvs and MB neurons will be one of the important subjects of our future studies.
Sleep is essential for the consolidation of Drosophila courtship memory [12,52]. In addition, Pdf-positive l-LNvs regulate light-dependent arousal [16], and knockdown of Ap and Chi in l-LNvs reduces the sleep amount [24]. These findings suggest that reduced sleep caused by Ap knockdown impairs memory consolidation. However, this is unlikely the case because Chi knockdown in Pdf neurons did not affect 1-day memory (Fig 6), even though it reduces the sleep amount [24]. Thus, the impairment of memory consolidation induced by Ap knockdown in l-LNvs does not merely result from reduced sleep.
We previously identified that the targeted expression of a CREB repressor in MB α/β neurons during the memory maintenance phase (48 to 72 hours after conditioning) impairs LTM [9], indicating that CREB-dependent transcription in MB α/β neurons is necessary for the maintenance of courtship LTM. Similarly, in this study, our results support the idea that Ap/Chi-dependent transcription in MB α/β neurons contributes to LTM maintenance for 2 days or longer. Taken together, courtship LTM is likely stored, at least in part, in MB α/β neurons from the second day after conditioning, and proteins required for maintaining LTM for 2 days or longer are possibly provided via transcriptions controlled by CREB and Ap/Chi. Although transcriptional regulations by CREB and Ap/Chi remain elusive, it will be of interest to investigate how the transcription by CREB and that by Ap/Chi are related and whether they occur in MB α/β neurons sequentially or in parallel.
The Pdf neuropeptide is necessary for the maintenance of courtship LTM, and the electrical silencing of Pdf neurons using Kir2.1 impairs LTM maintenance [9]. Furthermore, we found that the electrical silencing of Pdf neurons impairs memory consolidation (Fig 8E). Therefore, the electrical activity of Pdf neurons is essential for both memory consolidation and LTM maintenance. However, we found that the disruption of the Dynamin function using shits1 in Pdf neurons impairs memory consolidation but not LTM maintenance (Fig 8). These seemingly paradoxical consequences may result from the characteristic difference between the effects of Kir2.1 and mutated Dynamin (Shits1) on neurotransmission. The expression of shits1 in target neurons leads to the temperature-dependent blockage of synaptic vesicle recycling and thus synaptic neurotransmission [42]. On the other hand, the expression of Kir2.1 in target neurons suppresses the excitability of the target neurons. Consequently, neurotransmission from the target neurons should be blocked [53]. Although the Pdf neuropeptide is essential for the generation of locomotor activity rhythms in Drosophila [54], the disruption of the Dynamin function in Pdf neurons has little effect on locomotor activity rhythms [55]. However, the temporal induction of Kir2.1 in Pdf neurons can induce arrhythmic locomotor activity [56]. On the basis of these findings, it is considered that Kir2.1 blocks Pdf release, but Dynamin-dependent synaptic transmission has little effect on Pdf release. We previously identified by qRT-PCR analysis that Pdf neuron–specific Ap knockdown does not affect Pdf expression [24]. Furthermore, Pdf knockdown impairs LTM maintenance; nevertheless, it does not affect memory consolidation [9]. Considering these findings, it is unlikely that Pdf release from l-LNvs plays a significant role in memory consolidation. Overall, our findings raise the possibility that Dynamin-dependent synaptic transmission (e.g., classical neurotransmitter release) from l-LNvs contributes to memory consolidation to establish courtship LTM.
In Drosophila, the LTM maintenance phase has been conceptually defined as the time after LTM is fully formed and consolidated, and it is generally believed that memory consolidation is completed within 1 day after conditioning [1,2]. Hirano and colleagues have reported that the consolidation and maintenance phases are molecularly separated in aversive olfactory LTM [10]. Similarly, we have proposed that the maintenance phase in courtship LTM is also molecularly distinguished from the memory consolidation phase [9]. Because Pdf01 mutant flies display intact 1-day memory but impaired 2-day memory [9], it is possible that the maintenance phase can be distinguished from the consolidation phase on the basis of the requirement of the Pdf function. In this study, we found that apnull/Pdf-GAL4; UAS-ap/+ flies also show intact 1-day memory but impaired 2-day memory (Fig 5), suggesting that the consolidation and maintenance phases in courtship LTM are molecularly and cellularly separated.
Ap is evolutionarily conserved in vertebrates and invertebrates, and its function in regulating developmental events is also well conserved [17]. Lhx2, a mammalian ortholog of Ap, plays essential roles in neurodevelopmental events (e.g., cell proliferation, axon pathfinding, and neurite outgrowth) in the central nervous system [17,57,58]. In particular, Lhx2 is involved in the development of the mouse hippocampus, which is one of the crucial brain structures regulating learning and memory [57,59–62]. As was observed in Drosophila Ap, Lhx2 also continues to be expressed in the mature hippocampus [63], suggesting its role in hippocampus-dependent brain functions such as learning and memory. Although little is known about the functional significance of mammalian Ap orthologues in the adult brain, our findings raise the interesting possibility that they are also involved in memory processes.
[END]
[1] Url:
https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/article?id=10.1371/journal.pbio.3001459
(C) Plos One. "Accelerating the publication of peer-reviewed science."
Licensed under Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0)
URL:
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
via Magical.Fish Gopher News Feeds:
gopher://magical.fish/1/feeds/news/plosone/