(C) PLOS One [1]. This unaltered content originally appeared in journals.plosone.org.
Licensed under Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license.
url:https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/licenses-and-copyright

------------



An altered balance of integrated and segregated brain activity is a marker of cognitive deficits following sleep deprivation

['Nathan E. Cross', 'Perform Centre', 'Concordia University', 'Montreal', 'Center For Studies In Behavioral Neurobiology', 'Department Of Health', 'Kinesiology', 'Applied Physiology', 'Institut Universitaire De Gériatrie De Montréal', 'Criugm']

Date: 2021-11

Abstract Sleep deprivation (SD) leads to impairments in cognitive function. Here, we tested the hypothesis that cognitive changes in the sleep-deprived brain can be explained by information processing within and between large-scale cortical networks. We acquired functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scans of 20 healthy volunteers during attention and executive tasks following a regular night of sleep, a night of SD, and a recovery nap containing nonrapid eye movement (NREM) sleep. Overall, SD was associated with increased cortex-wide functional integration, driven by a rise of integration within cortical networks. The ratio of within versus between network integration in the cortex increased further in the recovery nap, suggesting that prolonged wakefulness drives the cortex towards a state resembling sleep. This balance of integration and segregation in the sleep-deprived state was tightly associated with deficits in cognitive performance. This was a distinct and better marker of cognitive impairment than conventional indicators of homeostatic sleep pressure, as well as the pronounced thalamocortical connectivity changes that occurs towards falling asleep. Importantly, restoration of the balance between segregation and integration of cortical activity was also related to performance recovery after the nap, demonstrating a bidirectional effect. These results demonstrate that intra- and interindividual differences in cortical network integration and segregation during task performance may play a critical role in vulnerability to cognitive impairment in the sleep-deprived state.

Citation: Cross NE, Pomares FB, Nguyen A, Perrault AA, Jegou A, Uji M, et al. (2021) An altered balance of integrated and segregated brain activity is a marker of cognitive deficits following sleep deprivation. PLoS Biol 19(11): e3001232. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3001232 Academic Editor: Simon Hanslmayr, University of Glasgow, UNITED KINGDOM Received: April 14, 2021; Accepted: September 28, 2021; Published: November 4, 2021 Copyright: © 2021 Cross et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Data Availability: All individual quantitative observations that underlie the data summarized in the figures are included in S1-S4 Data files. An explanation of this data and how it corresponds to each aspect of the figures is described in the documents ‘Data_underlying_figures.docx’, included with these files. All code used in this study was developed by the authors for the execution of the study, and is available online at https://github.com/nathanecross/sleep-deprivation. Raw data are restricted due to legal/ethical considerations. However, they may be shared with other investigators upon reasonable request and evaluation of such request by our local ethics review board: [email protected]. Funding: This research was supported by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (TDV) and the Canada Foundation for Innovation (TDV). The MRI compatible high-density EEG device (Philips Neuro) and data acquisition were made possible through an internal grant from PERFORM center and the Faculty of Arts and Science of Concordia University (CG). TDV is also supported by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (MOP 142191, PJT 153115, PJT 156125 and PJT 166167), the Fonds de Recherche du Québec – Santé and Concordia University. CG is supported Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada Discovery grants as well as the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (PJT-159948 and MOP-133619) and the Fonds de Recherche du Québec, Nature et Technology (research team grant). UA was supported by postdoctoral fellowships from Savoy Foundation and The Fonds de recherche du Quebec—Sante (FRQS). TDV has received consultant fees from Eisai, Paladin Labs and Jazz Pharmaceuticals. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist. Abbreviations: ANT, attention network task; BOLD, blood oxygen level dependent; CCER, Comité central d’éthique de la recherche; ECG, electrocardiography; EEG, electroencephalography; EOG, electrooculogram; FCR, functional clustering ratio; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; ICA, independent component analysis; i.i.d, independent and identically distributed; MCT, Mackworth clock task; NREM, nonrapid eye movement; PCA, principal component analysis; PRN, post recovery nap; ROI, region of interest; RW, rested wakefulness; SD, sleep deprivation; SWA, slow wave activity

Introduction The cognitive consequences of acute total sleep deprivation (SD) are substantial, negatively affecting a wide range of processes including attention, vigilance, and working memory [1,2]. Yet, the measurable impact of SD on cognition varies across individuals in a trait-like manner [3,4]. Investigating intraindividual changes in brain activity patterns has significant potential in understanding the interindividual effects of prolonged wakefulness on cognitive functioning. Previous studies have demonstrated the effects of acute total SD on brain activity measured with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during the resting state, such as disrupted connectivity within and between large-scale cortical networks [5] and a reduction in network modularity [6]. However, very few studies have investigated the changes in brain connectivity during the performance of cognitive tasks. Both localised brain regions and cortical networks display characteristic and independent (segregated) activity patterns during the performance of tasks [7,8]. However, even simple behaviours and cognitive processes require the coordination (integration) of information flows across systems distributed in the brain [9]. It has been proposed that efficient cognitive functioning is reflected through a balance of segregation and integration of information across brain networks [10,11]. Endogenous activity fluctuations in the human brain share a robust association with ongoing cognitive and perceptual processes [12,13], as they may interfere with the capacity for information processing. Importantly, both local and global fMRI activity fluctuates at a significantly greater magnitude in states of reduced arousal and vigilance [14], as well as following SD [5,15,16]. Therefore, in states of reduced arousal, an elevation in endogenous fluctuations across the cortex may affect the ability to integrate information between and within cortical networks, corresponding to the cognitive deficits experienced during these states. These deficits may also be driven by certain changes in subcortical regions, such as the thalamus, which is involved in both maintaining cortical arousal [17] and constraining the engagement of distributed neural assemblies within the cerebral cortex [18]. However, this has not been explored following experimental manipulation of arousal states, such as SD. Furthermore, SD-induced cognitive impairment partially improves with subsequent sleep [19]. Yet, it remains unclear whether this behavioural recovery is associated with a functional “recovery” of cortical networks after sleep. Here, we assessed the principles of functional brain activity across various arousal states using simultaneous electroencephalography (EEG)–fMRI recording and an SD protocol. Participants were scanned both at rest and while performing cognitive tasks probing attention, executive control, and working memory, after both a regular night of sleep and 24 hours of SD. Additionally, we measured EEG–fMRI activity during a recovery nap containing nonrapid eye movement (NREM) sleep following SD, as well as during a post recovery nap (PRN) session when the rest and task sequences were repeated. Firstly, to quantify the functional interactions within and between cortical networks across vigilance states, we computed a measure of integration of fMRI activity [20,21] within and between functional networks across different hierarchical levels of a representation of the cortex. We then compared the changes in functional integration during task performance to the magnitude of cognitive impairment following SD. In addition, we measured the association between recovery of both performance and the integration of cortical networks following the nap. Next, we assessed these changes in the context of changes to the amplitude of both local activity fluctuations and the global brain signal. Finally, we investigated the influence of thalamocortical activity on functional integration in the cortex across different arousal states.

Discussion These findings demonstrate that in states of low arousal (i.e., SD), the integration within cortical networks increases relative to integration between networks. These changes were observed during the performance of cognitive tasks, and, importantly, were strongly related to overall performance. The balance of these 2 integration terms, i.e., the FCR, can be viewed as a measure of the degree of functional segregation of a system into its constituent parts [11,20]. In other words, this signifies how much information cortical networks generate independently, compared with the information generated by the cortex as a whole. The FCR was found to increase across the cortex in the SD state and preferentially within certain functional networks (each divided, in turn, into smaller networks; Fig 2B), suggesting that these modifications in information integration were present when measured across different hierarchical levels of the cortex. We detected the greatest FCR during the NREM recovery nap (Fig 3C), consistent with previous observations during NREM sleep [20,31,32]. Although previous comparisons of NREM sleep were with the wakeful resting state, the current findings demonstrate that these effects generalise to other states of wakefulness (i.e., task performance). The increase in FCR was driven by both an increase in within-network integration as well as a significant decrease in between-network integration. It is widely accepted that sleep is not merely a state of quiescence, but an active brain state of self-organised, endogenous activity mostly cut off from the outside world. The level of consciousness has been hypothesised to be related to the degree of integrated information in the brain, that is, the information generated by the interactions in a whole system, beyond the information generated by the parts [33,34]. This is consistent with our observations of segregated brain activity during the recovery nap. Alternatively, in the SD state, the integration between networks remained the same as the RW state, which is coherent with the fact that subjects remained in a conscious, wakeful state. However, given the increase of within-network integration, the FCR was still significantly higher when compared to RW. In summary, these results suggest that following prolonged wakefulness, the proportional integration and segregation of brain activity within structured cortical networks appears to be driven from levels in well RW towards those observed during offline states, such as NREM sleep, suggesting that there exists an underlying continuum of functional segregation from RW to NREM sleep. We were not able to robustly measure the differences between NREM2 and NREM3 in this study with the same amount of consecutive data points as in the wake conditions; however, other studies suggest that brain networks are even more segregated during NREM3 compared to NREM2 sleep [35] and that total integration decreases in proportion to SWA [20]. We have recently shown that the primary axes of differentiation in functional connectivity (i.e., functional gradients) across the cortex do not undergo major changes following SD [36]. This is consistent with the current findings, although within-network integration increased, and between-network functional connectivity remained stable (Fig D in S1 Text). Furthermore, the gradient approach is mainly sensitive to the shape of the functional connectivity distributions rather than variations in amplitude. Indeed, in this study, we found that changes in the integration of brain activity are related to elevated amplitude of brain signal fluctuations, particularly the global signal (Fig 4F). We were able to confirm that this relationship was not due to increased motion during the SD state (SI Results in S1 Text). The reason behind an increase in signal amplitude during SD is not clear. However, one emerging candidate for a primary role of sleep is the flushing of metabolic waste through the mixing of cerebrospinal and interstitial fluid via the glymphatic system [37,38]. Large oscillations of fluid inflow into the perivascular space occur during sleep and are tightly coupled with large amplitude EEG slow waves [39,40]. Thus, a buildup of waste products from extended periods of cellular activity in the brain may trigger the mechanisms (i.e., large amplitude slow waves) for the glymphatic system to begin clearing waste, even in the wake state [38]. Additionally, it is also plausible that a change in respiration or cardiac activity due to parasympathetic drive could account for changes in BOLD activity amplitude, as cerebral blood flow increases after SD [41], and the greatest changes in integration were detected nearest to the middle cerebral artery [42]. Finally, given the assumptions of our model of integration, in particular that the BOLD data at each time point are temporally independent and identically distributed (i.i.d) realisations of a 400-dimensional random variable y [21], it is possible that potential sample dependencies across time could contribute to an overestimation of integration measures within the data. This could mean that the increased integration we observed in the SD condition is due to an increase in temporal dependencies in the data. This should be investigated in future studies investigating the dynamics of brain activity in the SD state. The effect of spatial dependence in the BOLD time series would have had a negligible impact on these findings, as it has been shown that this effect is important at the small spatial scale (i.e., few voxels) and is less important at the regional level (i.e., >30 mm), the latter of which was implemented in this study [43]. Our results demonstrate that disruption to the balance between integration and segregation of cortical networks has a significant impact on effective and efficient cognitive performance (Fig 3A), which was shown to be specific to executive attention and working memory rather than vigilance (Fig G in S1 Text). This effect was bidirectional, as both increased FCR following SD negatively correlated with performance change, while the decrease in FCR following a recovery nap was associated with the greatest cognitive improvement. Maintaining a balance between integration and segregation of information flow is thought to be crucial for distributed brain networks to execute effective cognitive function [7,11]. In particular, the ability to dynamically fluctuate between integrated and segregated brain states may be the primary mechanism that supports ongoing cognitive processes [44,45]. Information generated by the brain would theoretically decrease if dynamic states become more homogenous (indicated by an increase in total and within-network integration or global signal amplitude). Evidence from resting state fMRI studies have demonstrated that SD results in increased time spent in dynamic states associated with low vigilance [46–48]. Also, it has recently been shown that brain connectivity integration changes during resting state are coupled with arousal fluctuations [49]. However, how SD impacts state dynamics during the performance of cognitive tasks remains to be seen. We also replicated previous findings demonstrating that SD results in disruptive changes of thalamocortical functional connectivity, including the introduction of negative correlations [5,50]. The underlying mechanisms and physiological meaning of negative functional connectivity are still unclear [51–53]. However, the disruption of thalamocortical connectivity would have a significant impact on the maintenance of cortical arousal. The intralaminar and midline nuclei of the thalamus, with strong inputs from brainstem nuclei, are considered as part of the ascending reticular activating system that stimulate the cortex and facilitate the generation of intrinsic functional modes [17,54,55]. The thalamus also serves as the major hub for gating the flow of information into the cerebral cortex by blocking incoming signals through synaptic inhibition of thalamocortical relays. This is proposed to be the main mechanism contributing to shifting the brain from an aroused state into an “offline” state such as sleep [56]. However, we observed no relationship between the change in thalamocortical connectivity and cortical integration or cognitive performance across subjects (Fig 5C). Specifically, almost every subject expressed decreased (or anticorrelated) thalamocortical connectivity in the SD state, while there were more varied changes in integration between the RW and SD states across subjects (some not exhibiting relative increases in integration or FCR after 24 hours of SD). There are 2 possible explanations for these findings. Firstly, during prolonged wakefulness, thalamocortical disconnection may precede changes in cortical integration, the latter occurring after different durations of SD across individuals. This is supported by evidence that during sleep onset, thalamic deactivation (i.e., thalamic activity decreasing to overall sleep levels) precedes cortical changes by several minutes [57,58]. This would also be consistent with the observation that the thalamocortical connectivity was at its lowest in the SD resting state sequence (Fig 5D), when it was confirmed through EEG that the majority of subjects experienced brief entry into sleep. An alternative possibility is that disrupted thalamocortical connectivity is a state-based effect of SD, while altered cortical integration underlies interindividual differences in cognitive performance (or cortical “reserves”) following SD. To compare these hypotheses would require repeated measurements of brain activity across an extended period of prolonged wakefulness. Regardless, these results further elucidate the relationship between the thalamus and cortex, demonstrating that, in some individuals, the cortex can still maintain an optimal balance of integration and segregation required for cognitive processing despite becoming functionally disconnected from the thalamus. As has been proposed, the ascending modulatory transmitter systems mostly provide the necessary arousal to tune the state and excitability of the different parts of the cortex, which allow for the appropriate analysis of sensory information (i.e., cognitive processing and behavioural responses) [56]. Therefore, thalamocortical disruption following SD may not directly cause impaired cognition, but rather be a precipitating factor via altered functional integration in the cortex.

Conclusions In conclusion, SD appears to impact the balance of integration and segregation of brain activity. Whether these changes are completely neurogenic or arise from systemic processes involved in maintaining homeostasis of the cellular environment in the brain remain to be elucidated. However, they appear independent from conventional markers of homeostatic sleep pressure and changes in thalamocortical connectivity. Regardless, the disruption of information integration in the brain is significantly linked to the extent of cognitive impairment experienced by individuals following SD. Future perspectives should focus on why integration and segregation of brain activity is impaired following SD and how this impacts the dynamics of integrated network states during cognitive task performance.

Materials and methods Population and experimental design A total of 20 participants were recruited using advertisements posted online and within Concordia University, Montreal. This study was approved by the Comité central d’éthique de la recherche (CCER), established by the Ministère de la Santé et des Services sociaux in Quebec. This ethical review of research adheres to the principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki. Informed written consent was obtained from all participants. All participants were 18 to 30 years old, healthy, and regular sleepers, and none were taking any medication. Participants made 3 visits to the sleep laboratory, including a night for habituation and screening to rule out sleep disorders, a normal night (8-hour sleep, RW) and experimental night (0-hour sleep, SD). Each morning in the RW and SD conditions, participants completed one resting state sequence and 3 cognitive tasks: the ANT, MCT, and the N-back task. In the SD condition, participants were provided a 60-minute opportunity to sleep inside the MRI, and the tasks and then the resting state sequence were repeated. The order of the RW and SD sessions was counterbalanced across subjects (SI Materials and methods in S1 Text). Analysis The change in performance outcomes on each of the 3 separate cognitive tasks were grouped into either accuracy (% of correct responses) or speed (reaction time, in ms). This provided 2 data matrices of size #subjects × #tasks, one matrix containing accuracy scores and one containing reaction times. The change scores for accuracy or speed were entered into separate PCAs, in order to provide a global estimate of performance. The first principal component from each PCA was extracted and used as a single measure for accuracy or speed across the 3 tasks. fMRI data acquisition and analysis MRI scanning was acquired with a 3T GE scanner (General Electric Medical Systems, Wisconsin, United States) using an 8-channel head coil. Structural T1-weighted images with a 3D BRAVO sequence, and functional echo planar images were acquired. Data were preprocessed using the fMRIPrep [59] and xcpEngine [60] toolboxes (for detailed steps, see SI Materials and methods in S1 Text). The preprocessed BOLD time series for each subject was projected onto the cortical surface and smoothed along the surface using a 6-mm smoothing kernel using the FreeSurfer software package. For each subject and session, the general task-specific activity (convolved with a hemodynamic response function) was regressed out from the BOLD time series, and the time series from all tasks were concatenated into one time series per subject and condition. Network and assembly identification For all analyses of task data, the BOLD time series for each vertex on the fsaverage surface was assigned to one of 400 cortical parcels from a predetermined standardised template of functionally similar cortical regions [27]. The time series for all the vertices corresponding to each parcel were averaged to give one mean time series per parcel, resulting in a total of 400 time series across both cortical hemispheres. We then divided the cortex in 3 separate steps, which allowed us to arrange the data in a hierarchical framework comprising 4 levels (Fig B in S1 Text). Firstly, at the cortex level, each parcel was assigned to either 1 of 7 functional networks, taken from a template based on a large sample of resting state data [28]. At the second level of the hierarchy (7 networks), each parcel was assigned to 1 of 17 functional networks taken from a higher resolution version of the same template [28], and these 17 networks were clustered into 1 of the 7 original networks based on naming convention. At the third level (17 networks), each of the 17 functional networks were further partitioned into assemblies (clusters) based upon a hierarchical clustering technique, resulting in a partitioned composed of 57 clusters. To achieve this last level of the hierarchy, the averaged correlation matrix was computed for each network during the RW session and thresholded at p < 0.05. The thresholded correlation structure was computed for each of the 17 networks, and their structure was assessed by a hierarchical clustering that maximised intraclass similarity, defined as , where r is the correlation coefficient between 2 regions. By thresholding the similarity trees at the level of the highest increase of intracluster distance, the 17 networks were divided into assemblies of areas (Fig E in S1 Text). Subcortical regions were extracted from the FreeSurfer segmentation of the FSL MNI152 template represented by 8 regions of interest (ROIs) corresponding to the left and right hemispherical thalamus, striatum, hippocampus, and amygdala. Partition into community structure and analyses of resting state data are described in SI Materials and methods in S1 Text. Functional connectivity and integration The mean time course of each parcel or volume was correlated to the mean time courses of all other parcels, and the correlation matrices were z-scored. Significance was corrected at p < 0.05 using the false discovery rate method [61]. Functional integration was calculated as follows. The functional data were considered as N ROIs (parcels) characterised by their mean time courses y = (y 1 ,…,y N ) gathered into K systems (networks) defined as S = {S 1 ,…,S K }. For any partition of y, integration (or mutual information) can then defined as the Kullback–Leibler information divergence between the joint distribution p(y 1 ,…y K ) and the product of the marginal distributions of the N-dimensional fMRI BOLD time series y divided into K subsets, such that which can be rewritten as where H is the Shannon entropy measure [21]. For multivariate normal data with mean mu and covariance matrix Sigma, entropy can be computed as where |Sigma| refers to the determinant of the covariance matrix. As described in [21], the total integration (I Tot ) can be decomposed, according the organisation of regions into systems, as the sum of each system’s integration relative to its regions (within-system integration) and a between-system integration term or, alternatively, Integration was calculated at different levels in our proposed hierarchical model of the cortex. Firstly, integration was calculated across the whole cortex where N = 400 parcels were gathered into 7 networks. Secondly, for each of 7 networks separately, the N parcels were gathered into small subnetworks (Yeo 17 networks), where each of the 7 networks contained a minimum of 2 out of 17 subnetworks. Finally, for each of the 17 subnetworks, the N parcels were divided into K of the 57 clusters (assemblies), where each of the 17 networks contained a minimum of 2 out of 57 clusters. We defined the FCR as the ratio of the integration within subsystems (I ws ), compared to the integration present between these subsystems (I bs ) [20]. It is a measure of clustering inside a given system because an increase in FCR indicates that subsystems become proportionally more independent of each other. In other words, it quantifies the degree of functional segregation of a given system into subsystems: The FCR was also computed for each network or cluster at each level of the hierarchy. EEG data acquisition and analysis EEG was acquired using an MR compatible 256 high-density geodesic sensor EEG array (Electrical Geodesics (Eugene, OR, USA), Magstim EGI). EEG data were recorded at 1,000 Hz referenced to Cz using a battery-powered MR-compatible 256-channel amplifier. Electrocardiography (ECG) was also collected via 2 MR compatible electrodes through a bipolar amplifier (Physiobox (Eugene, OR, USA), Magtism EGI). The EEG data were corrected for MR gradient ballistocardiographic pulse-related artefacts using the Brainvision Analyzer (Brain Products, Gilching, Germany). The MR-denoised EEG signal was band-pass filtered between 1 and 20 Hz to remove low-frequency drift and high-frequency noise, down-sampled to 250 Hz, and re-referenced to the linked mastoids. Scoring of the sleep session was performed in conjunction by 2 trained scorers (NEC and AAP) using the Wonambi toolbox (https://github.com/wonambi-python/wonambi) (using the channels Fz, F3, F4, C3, C4, O1, and O2) in order to obtain measures of sleep including total sleep time and the duration of sleep stages. For the analysis of wake EEG, eye blink artefacts were removed with independent component analysis (ICA) using the MNE Python package. The ICA was fit on each EEG time series concatenated across tasks for each subject and condition. The number of components was set to 15, with a random seed to ensure the uniformity of components. The resulting ICA components were then compared to electrooculogram (EOG) electrodes any independent components (ICs) that matched the EOG pattern were automatically marked for exclusion. Any matching components were then excluded from the original signal, and this new time series was then used for all subsequent analysis. Power spectral analysis was also computed using MNE, via the Welch method using a frequency resolution of 1 Hz. Statistical analysis Bayesian sampling scheme. Probable values of integration and FCR were inferred from the data using a Bayesian numerical sampling scheme that approximates the posterior distribution of the parameters of interest [21]. We first ran Gibbs sampling on the model to propose a numerical approximation of p(Sigma|y). Using this sampling procedure, we obtained 1,000 samples from the posterior distribution of the group covariance matrix (Sigmas = 1,…,1,000) in either condition (RW, SD, and PRN). From each sample Sigmas, we computed the corresponding values of integration (Iws and Ibs) and FCR. For each measure and condition, we therefore obtained a frequency histogram that, by construction, approximated the posterior distribution of that measure given the data. The samples were then used to provide approximations of either statistics (e.g., mean and SD) or probabilities (e.g., probability of an increase between RW and SD) approximated as the frequency of that increase observed in the samples. This also allowed to approximate the posterior probability p(A|y) of the assertion, e.g., FCR SD > FCR RW using the equation where # stands for the cardinal function of a set. For analyses between conditions, a probability of difference >0.95 was considered significant. During the sampling procedure, a covariance matrix was also estimated for each individual. Thus, similarly to the group-level analysis, we compared the resulting estimates of FCR at the individual level with other individual metrics (e.g., performance measures, global signal fluctuation, and thalamocortical connectivity) using product–moment correlations. Spatial correspondence between cortical maps. For the comparison between brain maps (Fig 3), we used a spatial permutation framework to generate null models of overlap [62]. Spatial permutation of brain maps was performed using 100 angular permutations of spherical projections of the cortical surface. This approach calculates a correspondence statistic between 2 maps while controlling for spatial autocorrelation in the data.

Supporting information S1 Text. This file contains Supporting information Materials and methods and Results that provide more detailed information regarding how the study was conducted and control analyses that provide more information surrounding the results reported in the main text. This file also contains Supporting information Tables A–H and Figs A–H. Table A: Mean task performance scores in each state. Table B: The I Tot within 7 cortical networks, in the WR, SD, and PRN states. Table C: The I Tot within 17 cortical networks, in the WR, SD, and PRN states. Table D: The FCR within 7 cortical networks, in the WR, SD, and PRN states. Table E: The FCR within 17 cortical networks, in the WR, SD, and PRN states. Table F: Changes in total integration and FCR across the whole cortex and cortical networks, when task-specific activity was not regressed out of the time series for each parcel. Table G: Changes in total integration and FCR across the whole cortex using networks defined by the Louvain modularity algorithm. Table H: Mean sleep statistics during the nap. Fig A: Performance outcomes (accuracy + reaction time) across all tasks were significantly impaired following SD and improved following the recovery nap. Fig B: (A) The matrix of change in resting state functional correlations between the time series of 100 cortical parcels depicts a widespread increase in functional connectivity from WR to SD states, however, to a lesser extent than during tasks (unthresholded, lower triangle; thresholded P FDR <0.05, upper triangle). (B) Changes in integration are shown across 2 levels of a hierarchical model of the cortex: whole cortex and 7 networks (the other levels could not be calculated with a 100 parcellation templat, due to spatial resolution and that integration calculations required >1 parcels per region). The total integration within the cortex increased from the WR to the SD state and within 6 out of the 7 functional networks (white italics depict significant changes, variations probability >0.95). (C) The change in total integration mapped onto the cortical surface illustrates a widespread increase in integration across the entire cortex during resting state. Fig C: Each of the 17 Yeo functional networks were partitioned into assemblies (clusters) based upon a hierarchical clustering that maximised intraclass similarity. A total whole brain partition of 57 clusters was resulting from this procedure. To achieve this, the averaged correlation matrix was computed for each network during the WR session and thresholded at p < 0.05. The thresholded correlation structure was computed for each of the 17 networks, and their structure was assessed by a hierarchical clustering that maximised intraclass similarity, defined as , where r is the correlation coefficient between 2 regions. By thresholding the similarity trees at the level of the highest increase of intracluster distance (dotted line), the 17 networks were divided into assemblies of areas. Clusters are displayed upon the cortical surface (upper right). Fig D: There was a significant increase in total integration from RW to SD. This increase was due to an increase in within-systems (networks) integration, with no change in integration between networks. Alternatively, while there was no change in total integration during the NREM nap, there was a significant increase in within-systems and decrease in between-systems integration (solid lines represent significant differences between states). Fig E: (A) Five networks (communities) were detected from the functional connectome in the WR condition using the Louvain modularity algorithm of the Brain Connectivity Toolbox. Visually, these appear to cover well described cortical networks in the literature: visual; somatomotor; default mode; limbic; attentional networks. (B) When hierarchical integration was computed across these extracted networks, there was an observed increase from WR to the SD condition across the level of the entire cortex and also within 4 of the 5 networks (*bold italics represent significant differences across states). Fig F: (A) Associations between the increase in total integration and cognitive performance. Similar to the FCR, there was a significant negative relationship between change in integration and accuracy and a positive relationship with speed performance from RW to SD. (B) There was also a significant negative relationship between change in integration following the PRN and the change in performance from SD to PRN. Fig G: The change in performance from RW to SD on each task separately in comparison to the change in integration of cortical BOLD activity during each task. A greater increase in total integration was significantly related to worse accuracy and speed in the ANT and Nback tasks, but not the MCT. Fig H: (A) Overall a decrease in functional connectivity was observed from the SD to the PRN state. (B) Changes in integration are shown across different levels of a hierarchical model of the cortex: whole cortex, 7 networks, 17 networks, and 57 clusters. The total integration within the cortex decreased minimally from the WR to the SD state and at the network level decreases were only observed within the default mode, somatomotor and visual networks (bold white italics depict significant changes, FDR corrected). ANT, attention network task; BOLD, blood oxygen level dependent; FCR, functional clustering ratio; MCT, Mackworth clock task; NREM, nonrapid eye movement; P FDR , positive false discovery rate; PRN, post recovery nap; RW, rested wakefulness; SD, sleep deprivation; WR, well rested. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3001232.s001 (DOCX) S1 Data. This file contains data underlying Fig 2. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3001232.s002 (ZIP) S2 Data. This file contains data underlying Fig 3. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3001232.s003 (ZIP) S3 Data. This file contains data underlying Fig 4. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3001232.s004 (ZIP) S4 Data. This file contains data underlying Fig 5. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3001232.s005 (ZIP)

[END]

[1] Url: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/article?id=10.1371/journal.pbio.3001232

(C) Plos One. "Accelerating the publication of peer-reviewed science."
Licensed under Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0)
URL: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/


via Magical.Fish Gopher News Feeds:
gopher://magical.fish/1/feeds/news/plosone/