SUBJECT: THE HILL ABDUCTION CASE                             FILE: UFO 2707




PART 6



   ----------------------------------------------------------------------

   REPLY:  By David R. Saunders

     Last month,  Steven Soter and Carl Sagan offered two counterarguments
   relating to Terence Dickinson's article,  "The Zeta Reticuli  Incident"
   (ASTRONOMY, December 1974).

     Their  first argument was to observe that the inclusion of connecting
   lines  in  certain  maps  "is what a lawyer  would  call  'leading  the
   witness'."  This was used as the minor premise in a syllogism for which
   the major premise was never stated. Whether we should consider "leading
   the witness" a sin or not will depend on how we conceive the purpose of
   the  original article.  The implied analogy between ASTRONOMY  magazine
   and  a court of law is tenuous at best;  an expository article  written
   for a nonprofessional audience is entitled, in my opinion, to do all it
   can to facilitate communication -- assuming that the underlying message
   is honest.  Much of what we call formal education is really little more
   than  "leading  the witness",  and no one who accepts  the  educational
   goals objects very strongly to this process.  In this context,  we  may
   also  observe that Soter's and Sagan's first argument provides  another
   illustrative  example of "leading the witness";  the  argument  attacks
   procedure,   not  substance --  and serves only to blunt  the  reader's
   possible criticism of the forthcoming second argument.  This  paragraph
   may  also be construed as an effort to lead the witness.  Once we  have
   been  sensitized to the possibilities,  none of us needs to be  further
   misled!

     The  second  argument  offered  by Soter  and  Sagan  does  attack  a
   substance.   Indeed,   the editorial decision to publish  the  original
   article  was a responsible decision only if the issues raised  by  this
   second  line  of possible argument were fully considered.   Whenever  a
   statistical  inference  is made from selected data,  it is  crucial  to
   determine the strenuousness of that selection and then to appropriately
   discount the apparent clarity of the inference. By raising the issue of
   the possible effects of selection, Soter and Sagan are right on target.
   However, by failing to treat the matter with quantitative objectivity (
   by  failing  to weigh the evidence in each direction numerically,   for
   example), they might easily perform a net disservice.

     In some  situations,   the  weight of the appropriate  discount  will
   suffice to cancel the  clarity of a proposed inference --  and we  will
   properly dismiss the proposal as a mere capitalization on chance,  or a
   lucky outcome.  (It is abundantly clear that Soter and Sagan regard the
   star map results as  just  such a  fortuitous  outcome.)  In some other
   situations, the weight of the appropriate discount may be fully applied
   without  accounting for the clarity of the inference as  a  potentially
   valid  discovery.   For  example,   if I proposed to  infer  from  four
   consecutive  coin tosses observed as heads that the coin  would  always
   yield heads, you would properly dismiss this proposal as unwarranted by
   the data. However, if I proposed exactly the same inference based on 40
   similar  consecutive observations of heads,  you would almost certainly
   accept  the inference and begin looking with me for a  more  systematic
   explanation  of  the data.  The crucial difference here is  the  purely
   quantitative  distinction  between  4 and 40;  the two  situations  are
   otherwise   identical  and  cannot  be  distinguished  by  any   purely
   qualitative argument.

     When   Soter  and  Sagan  use  phrases  such  as  "some  subset  that
   resembles",  "free also to select the vantage point", "simple matter to
   optimize",  and "freedom to contrive a resemblance",  they are speaking
   qualitatively   about  matters  that  should  (and  can)   be   treated
   quantitatively. Being based only on this level of argument, Soter's and
   Sagan's conclusions can only be regarded as inconclusive.

     A complete quantitative examination of this problem will require  the
   numerical estimation of at least three factors, and their expression in
   a  uniform  metric  so that wee can see which way  the  weight  of  the
   evidence is leaning.  The most convenient common metric will be that of
   "bits  of  information",  which is equivalent to  counting  consecutive
   heads in the previous example.

     One key factor is the degree of resemblance between the Hill map  and
   the   optimally   similar  computer-drawn  map.   Precisely  how   many
   consecutive  heads  is this resemblance equivalent to?  A   second  key
   factor  is the precise size of the population of stars from  which  the
   computer was allowed to make its selection.  And a third key factor  is
   the  precise dimensionality of the space in which the computer was free
   to  choose the best vantage point.  If the first factor exceeds the sum
   of the other two by a sufficient margin,  we are justified in insisting
   on a systematic explanation for the data.

     The third factor is the easiest to deal with.  The dimensionality  of
   the  vantage-point  space is not more than three.  A  property  of  the
   metric  system for weighing evidence is that each independent dimension
   of  freedom leads us to expect the equivalent of one  more  consecutive
   head  in  the  observed data.  Three dimensions of  freedom  are  worth
   exactly  3.0   bits.   In  the end,  even three bits will  be  seen  as
   relatively minor.

     The  second  factor  might  be much larger than  this,   and  deserve
   relatively more discussion. The appropriate discount for this selection
   will be log2C,  where C is the number of distinct combinations of stars
   "available" to the computer.  If we were to agree that C must represent
   the possible combinations of 46  stars taken 14  at a time,  then log2C
   would  be 37.8  bits;  this would be far more than enough to  kill  the
   proposed  inference.  However,  not all these combinations are  equally
   plausible.   We  really  should consider  only  combinations  that  are
   adjacent  to one another and to the sun,  but it is awkward to  try  to
   specify exactly which combinations these are.

     The  really exciting moment in working with these data came with  the
   realization  that  in the real universe,  our sun belongs to  a  closed
   cluster  together with just six of the other admissible stars  --   Tau
   Ceti,  82  Eridani, Zeta Tucanae,  Alpha Mensae,  and Zeta 1 and Zeta 2
   Reticuli. The real configuration of interstellar distances is such that
   an  explorer  starting from any of the seven should visit all  of  them
   before  venturing  outside.  If the Hill map is assumed to include  the
   sun, then it should include the other members of this cluster within an
   unbroken  network of connections,  and the other connected stars should
   be relatively adjacent in the real universe.

     Zeta  Reticuli occupies a central position in all of  the  relatively
   few combinations that now remain plausible. However, in my opinion, the
   adjacency  criteria  do  leave some remnant  ambiguity  concerning  the
   combination  of  real stars to be matched against the Hill map --   but
   only with respect to the region farthest from the sun. The stars in the
   closed  cluster  and those in the chain leading to Gliese 67   must  be
   included,   as  well as Gliese 86  and two others from a  set  of  five
   candidates.  Log2C for this remnant selection is 3.9 bits. we must also
   notice  that  the  constraint that Zeta Tucanae  be  occulted  by  Zeta
   Reticuli reduces the dimensionality of the vantage-point space from 3.0
   to 1.0. Thus, the sum of factors two and three is now estimated as only
   4.9 bits.

     The first factor is also awkward to evaluate --  simply because there
   is no standard statistical technique for comparing points on two  maps.
   Using  an approximation based on rank-order correlation,  I've  guessed
   that the number we seek here is between 11 and 16.  (This is the result
   cited by Dickinson on page 15  of the original article.)  Deducting the
   second  and  third  factors,  this rough analysis  leaves  us  with  an
   empirical  result whose net meaning is equivalent to observing at least
   6  to 11 consecutive heads. (I say "at least",  because there are other
   factors  contributing to the total picture --  not discussed either  by
   Dickinson  or by Soter and Sagan --  that could be adduced  to  enhance
   this  figure.   For  example,  the computed vantage point  is  in  good
   agreement  with Betty Hill's reported position when observing the  map,
   and  the coordinate system implicit in the boundaries of the map is  in
   good agreement with a natural galactic coordinate system.  Neither have
   we discussed any quantitative use of the connections drawn on the  Hill
   map, which were put there in advance of any of these analyses.)

     In the final interpretation, it will always be possible to argue that
   5 or 10 or even 15  bits of remarkable information simply isn't enough.
   However,   this is a matter for each of us to decide independently.  In
   deciding  this matter,  it is more important that we be consistent with
   ourselves (as we review a large number of uncertain interpretations  of
   data that we have made) than that we be in agreement with some external
   authority.  I do believe, though,  that relatively few individuals will
   continue  a  coin-tossing  match in which  their  total  experience  is
   equivalent  to even six consecutive losses.  In scientific matters,  my
   own standard is that I'm interested in any result that has five or more
   bits  of information supporting it --  though I prefer not to stick  my
   neck  out  publicly  on the basis of less than 10.   Adhering  to  this
   standard,   I   continue  to  find the  star  map  results  exceedingly
   interesting.

   Dr.   David  R.  Saunders is a Research Associate at the University  of
   Chicago's Industrial Relations Center.




**********************************************
* THE U.F.O. BBS - http://www.ufobbs.com/ufo *
**********************************************